Focus on Facilitated Communication: An analysis of international scientific approaches towards a controversial method

Markus Scholz and Jan Markus Stegkemper

Abstract

Facilitated Communication (FC) has ever since been a controversial subject of discussion. But scientific research on the method declined over the years. Nevertheless, recent publication activities are bringing the topic back on the agenda. The goal of the presented study is to identify published studies dealing with FC and to analyze taken research approaches towards the method and their longitudinal development. Therefore, a content analysis of 90 publications was carried out. Results show prevailing but over time varying approaches towards FC. Within dominant strategies (e.g. validation using message passing) certain aspects (e.g. facilitated use of pictorial symbols) are often not examined. Furthermore, we have identified only few studies focusing on prevalence. Based on our outcomes, possible implications for future research are discussed.

A never ending story?

Facilitated Communication (FC), developed by Rosemary Crossley in the late 1970s, has been controversially discussed ever since. Over the years two factions of researchers, professionals and parents were emerging, either they were outright for or against the method. Still the discussion is reignited on a regular basis (ISAAC, 2014), but as Mostert states scientific research on the method declined over the years (2012, p. 20). Nevertheless, recent publication activities are bringing the topic back on the agenda.

FC research a short review

Since the 1990s a lot of studies have been conducted, mostly dealing with the validation of the method. Published reviews show that those studies mostly utilized message-passing as a method to prove whether FC is working or not (see Köhler & Scholz, 2014, p. 133). At the end of the 1990s, the scientific interest seemed to decline (see Mostert, 2010, p. 39) and other approaches to validate FC were used (Bober, 2011, p. 419).

Research Questions

Based on the literature review during a previous study we found that there are very different scientific approaches towards FC and that these seem to vary and change dynamically over time (Köhler & Scholz, 2014, p. 136). These lead us to the following questions:

1. How many studies have been published in the context of FC since 1990?

2. What kinds of methods and approaches are used?

3. How did the application of different approaches develop over time?

4. What kind of symbols are utilized for communication in the studies (written text, pictorial symbols, concrete objects, etc.)?

Data collection

The databases ERIC and FIS Bildung were used to retrieve all publications published between 1990 and 2014. This initial data collection was conducted by our student assistant. Search terms were “Facilitated Communication” and the German equivalent “Gestützte Kommunikation”. We identified 160 articles in ERIC and another 66 articles in FIS Bildung. All bibliographic information including abstracts was gathered and publications using the term “Facilitated Communication” in a different understanding (not meaning the method of Crossley) were excluded.

We then utilized research reviews (Bober, 2010; Biermann, 1999; Green, 1994; Mostert, 2001; 2010; Probst, 2005) to supplement the initial database search , which added another 36 publications.

Afterwards we identified all empirical studies using a two rater validation based on a broad understanding of the term study:

1. Research interest: there is a research question or interest mentioned

2. Sample: an object of investigation is mentioned

3. Method/design: there is a description or documentation of a (methodological) procedure

4. Outcomes/results: some kinds of results or outcomes are mentioned

All publications were successively coded by two raters. Afterwards intercoder agreement was measured in percent (95.2%). To ensure a clear sample, we conducted a discursive clarification of all differently rated segments until we reached full agreement. In the end, 90 publications identified as primary studies were used for our analysis (see references).

Analysis

All studies were categorized in a deductive, concept-driven coding process (Gibbs, 2007, p. 44-45) using MAXQDA. Our previous sorting (Köhler & Scholz, 2014) was utilized as coding schedule. To ensure a clear assignment of codes a corresponding coding manual was written (Bryman, 2012, p. 298-299). Variables in MAXQDA were used to record the year and type of each publication and the location in which a study was conducted. This enabled us to count and visualize different scientific approaches taken towards FC on a timeline and to discuss how these approaches quantitatively varied.

To determine how many studies were conducted we had to distinguish between the publications and the study itself. If there was at least one similar author, we compared the reported samples, to identify multiple publications of the same outcomes. Only the study itself was counted.

We used the same procedure mentioned above, to ensure the reliability of our categorization.

Additional data-driven analysis

In a second step, we jointly revisited all pre-sorted studies in an inductive, more data-driven way (Gibbs, 2007, p. 45) to define sub-categories. The categories and sub-categories were then used to develop a visualization of taken research approaches.

Outcomes

We could identify a total of 90 studies focusing FC (see references). Most were published as journal articles (76.7%), followed by book sections (14.4%), web pages (5.6%) and full books (3.3%). Most studies were conducted in the United States (52.2%) (see table I).

Table 1
Countries of origin of studies (n=90)

Country of Origin

USA

Germany

Australia

Canada

Finland

47(52.2%)

16(17.8%)

7(7.8%)

6(6.7%)

4(4.4%)

Country of Origin

UK

Italy

Japan

Denmark

4(4.4%)

4(4.4%)

1(1.1%)

1(1.1%)


The high percentage of German publications can be explained by our sampling procedure. The majority of studies used a quantitative approach (68.9%), 27.8% applied qualitative methods. Combinations of quantitative and qualitative methods were found in 3.3% of the studies. We could identify five main approaches (see figure I).

Figure I. Approaches towards Facilitated Communication (N = 90)


Note. Some studies use more than one approach.

Most studies on the question of validation used message-passing (n = 50), text analyses were conducted in lesser extent (n = 9). The second most common approach focuses on the interactional process between the facilitator and the person who uses FC (n = 21). Studies considering practical use (n = 9) or prevalence (n = 5) were found in lesser extent. A relatively high number of studies were categorized as “other” (n = 19), their contents are taken into account in figure IV.

The application of the different approaches greatly varies over time. Until the year 1999 message-passing-studies were the dominant way of looking at FC (see figure II).

Figure II. Development of the application of different approaches towards Facilitated Communication (N = 90)


Note. Some studies use more than one approach.

Publications utilizing this method rose until the mid-1990s, afterwards they strongly decreased up to the change of the millennium. From 1999 to 2008, studies on FC in general declined. In recent years the scientific interest reemerged. Also more diverse approaches can be seen.

FC is mostly examined as a method based on the usage of written language (n = 83). Pictures, pictorial symbols or concrete objects are often not taken into account (see figure III).

Figure III. Understanding of Facilitated Communication based on the used symbols (N = 90)


Note. Some studies use more than one kind of symbols.

Our data driven approach enabled us to structure the versatile scientific approaches. The studies focus either on (1) one or multiple single settings involving a facilitator and/or a person who uses FC or on (2) one or multiple settings on larger scales (e.g. prevalence) (see figure IV).

Figure IV. Visualization of scientific approaches taken towards Facilitated Communication


The approaches focusing (multiple) single settings, are (1) studies regarding the person who uses FC and his or her social environment (e.g. Lang, 2003; Probst, 2012b). (2) Studies focusing the facilitators (e.g. Bayer, Janz, & Klauß, 2010; Perry, Bryson, & Bebko, 1998; Sipilä & Määttä, 2011). (3) Most approaches raise the question if FC works and take a closer look at the “process” or the “products” of FC processes without a presupposition, e.g. using text analysis. In this regard there are also studies which a priori assume that the method is working and analyze the meaning of statements (e.g. Ashby & Causton-Theoharis, 2012; Biklen, 1997).

Summary and possible implications for future research

Approaches towards FC are methodically diverse although quantitative aspects are dominating. Not surprisingly most studies deal with the validation of the method. The number of message-passing-studies has strongly declined over the years, while especially in recent years a broader variety of methods and questions have been assessed. It seems as if researchers are trying to occupy blank spots in the field. Moreover, most studies understand FC as a method based on written language. Different aspects, like the use of concrete objects, pictures or pictorial symbols are far less considered.

Based on our findings we would like to stress three aspects, which from our point of view are important: First, by strongly focusing on questions of validation and process analyses other things have long been overlooked. It is mandatory to regard that – despite its questionable validity – the method is used in practical situations, presumably all over the world (see Lilienfeld, Marshall, Todd, & Shane, 2014, p.73). Lilienfeld et al. pointed out the danger related to this “science-practice gap” (2014, p. 65). Second, the relevance of written language in FC is not as big as the number of studies solely focusing on this aspect would suggest. Third, the gap spotting strategy and concomitant new approaches are making the field more unclear and perhaps prevent a discussion between different factions. A questionable method which is presumably used in apparent numbers must also be accompanied by questions relevant to the practical field.

References

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the analysis

*Ashby, C. E., & Causton-Theoharis, J. (2012). "Moving quietly through the door of opportunity": Perspectives of College Students who Type to Communicate. Equity & Excellence in Education, 45(2), 261-282.

*Baldac, S., & Parsons, C. (1997). Factors Affecting Performance in Facilitated Communication. In D. Biklen & D. N. Cardinal (Eds.), Contested Words, Contested Science. Unraveling the Facilitated Communication controversy (pp. 79-95). New York: Teachers' College Press.

*Bara, B. G., Bucciarelli, M., & Colle, L. (2001). Communicative Abilities in Autism: Evidence for Attentional Deficits. Brain and Language, 77(2), 216-240.

*Barabash, M. K. (1993). Using Facilitated Communication To Improve Yes/No Responses in Elementary Age Non-Verbal Severly and Profoundly Mentally Handicapped Cerebral Palsied Students. Master's Practicum Report, Nova University. Retrieved on 06.10.2014 from http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED361985.pdf

*Bayer, M., Janz, F., & Klauß, T. (2010). Erfahrungen und Meinungen zur Facilitated Communication (FC). In A. Alfaré, T. Huber-Kaiser, F. Janz, & T. Klauß (Eds.), Facilitated Communication. Forschung und Praxis im Dialog (pp. 172-200). Karlsruhe: Von Loeper.

*Bebko, J., Perry, A., & Bryson, S. (1996). Multiple Method Validation Study of Facilitated Communication: II. Individual Differences and Subgroup Results. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(1), 19-42.

*Beck, A. R., & Pirovano, C. M. (1996). Facilitated Communicators' Performance on a Task of Receptive Language. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(5), 497-512.

*Bienstein, P., & Nußbeck, S. (2006). Unterstützte Kommunikation bei selbstverletzendem Verhalten aus Mitarbeitersicht - eine Erhebung in Wohneinrichtungen der Behindertenhilfe. Heilpädagogische Forschung, 32(2), 58-69.

Biermann, A. (1999). Gestützte Kommunikation im Widerstreit. Empirische Aufarbeitung eines umstrittenen Ansatzes. Berlin: Volker Spiess.

*Bigozzi, L., Zanobini, M., Tarchi, C., Cozzani, F., & Camba, R. (2012). Facilitated communication and autistic children: the problem of authorship. Life Span and Disability, 15(2), 55-74.

*Biklen, D. (1990). Communication Unbound: Autism and Praxis. Harvard Educational Review, 60(3), 291-314.

*Biklen, D. (1997). The Multiple Meanings of Independence: Perspectives from Facilitated Communication. In D. Biklen & D. N. Cardinal (Eds.), Contested Words, Contested Science. Unraveling the Facilitated Communication controversy (pp. 157-172). New York: Teachers' College Press.

*Biklen, D., Morton, M. W., Gold, D., Berrigan, C., & Swaminathan, S. (1992). Facilitated communication: Implications for individuals with autism. Topics in Language Disorders, 12(4), 1-28.

*Biklen, D., Morton, M. W., Saha, S. N., Duncan, J., Gold, D., Hardardottir, M., . . . Rao, S. (1991). "I AMN NOT A UTISTIVC ON THJE TYP" ("I'm not Autistic on the Typewriter"). Disability, Handicap & Society, 6(3), 161-180.

*Biklen, D., Saha, N., & Kliewer, C. (1995). How Teachers Confirm the Authorship of Facilitated Communication: A Portfolio Approach. The Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 20(1), 45-56.

*Biklen, D., & Schubert, A. (1991). New Words: The Communication of Students with Autism. Remedial and Special Education, 12(6), 46-57.

*Bligh, S., & Kupperman, P. (1993). Brief Report:Facilitated Communication Evaluation Procedure Accepted in a Court Case. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(3), 553-557.

Bober, A. (2010). Zur Wirkungsweise der körperlichen Stütze während der Gestützten Kommunikation (FC). Analyse des Forschungsstands und Ableitung weiterführender Forschungsfragen. Dissertation. Retrieved on 11.12.2013 from http://kups.ub.uni-koeln.de/3336/1/Bober_Dissertation_Druckfassung.pdf

Bober, A. (2011). Gestützte Kommunikation (FC). Forschungsstand zu sprachlichen Indikatoren der Autorenschaft. In H. Bollmeyer, K. Engel, A. Hallbauer, & M. Hüning-Meier (Eds.), UK inklusive. Teilhabe durch Unterstützte Kommunikation (pp. 418-433). Karlsruhe: von Loeper.

*Bomba, C., O'Donnell, L., Markowitz, C., & Holmes, D. L. (1996). Evaluating the Impact of Facilitated Communication on the Communicative Competence of Fourteen Students with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(1), 43-58.

*Braman, B. J., Brady, M. P., Linehan, S. L., & Williams, R. E. (1995). Facilitated Communication for Children with Autism: An Examination of Face Validity. Behavioral Disorders, 21(1), 110-119.

Bryman, A. (2012). Social Research Methods (4th ed.). Oxford: University Press.

*Bundschuh, K., & Basler-Eggen, A. (2000). Gestützte Kommunikation (FC) bei Menschen mit schweren Kommunikationsbeeinträchtigungen. München: Bayerisches Staatsministerium für Arbeit und Sozialordnung, Familie, Frauen und Gesundheit.

*Burgess, C. A., Kirsch, I., Shane, H., Niederauer, K. L., Graham, S. M., & Bacon, A. (1998). Facilitated communication as an ideomotor response. Psychological Science, 9(1), 71-74.

*Cabay, M. (1994). Brief Report: A Controlled Evaluation of Facilitated Communication Using Open-Ended and Fill-in Questions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(4), 517-527.

*Calculator, S. N., & Singer, K. M. (1992). Letter to the editor: Preliminary validation of facilitated communication. Topics in Language Disorders, 13(1), ix-xvi.

*Cardinal, D. N., Hanson, D., & Wakeham, J. (1996). Investigation of Authorship in Facilitated Communication. Mental Retardation, 34(4), 231-242.

*Cardinal, D. N., Hanson, D., & Wakeham, J. (1997). Who's Doing the Typing?: An Experimental Study. In D. Biklen & D. N. Cardinal (Eds.), Contested Words, Contested Science. Unraveling the Facilitated Communication controversy (pp. 34-53). New York: Teachers' College Press.

*Crews Jr., W. D., Sanders, E. C., Hensley, L. G., Johnson, Y. M., Bonaventura, S., Rhodes, R. D., & Garren, M. P. (1995). An Evaluation of Facilitated Communication in a Group of Nonverbal Individuals with Mental Retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25(2), 205-213.

*Crossley, R. (1992). Getting the words out: Case studies in facilitated communication training. Topics in Language Disorders, 12(4), 46-59.

*Eberlin, M., Ibel, S., & Jacobson, J. W. (1994). The Source of Messages Produced During Facilitated Communication with a Boy with Autism and Severe Mental Retardation. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 19(6), 657-671.

*Eberlin, M., McConnachie, G., Ibel, S., & Volpe, L. (1993). Facilitated communication: A failure to replicate the phenomenon. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(3), 507-530.

*Edelson, S. M., Rimland, B., Berger, C. L., & Billings, D. (1998). Evaluation of a Mechanical Hand Support for Facilitated Communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(2), 153-157.

*Emerson, A. (2010). Analyse der bei FC verwendeten Wörter als Indikator für Autorenschaft und Einflussnahme bei der Gestützten Kommunikation. In A. Alfaré, T. Huber-Kaiser, F. Janz, & T. Klauß (Eds.), Facilitated Communication. Forschung und Praxis im Dialog (pp. 44-50). Karlsruhe: Von Loeper.

*Emerson, A., Grayson, A., & Griffiths, A. (2001). Can't or won't? Evidence relating to authorship in facilitated communication. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36(1), 98-103.

Gibbs, G. R. (2007). Analyzing Qualitative Data. Los Angeles: Sage.

*Grayson, A., Emerson, A., Howard-Jones, P., & O'Neil, L. (2012). Hidden communicative competence: Case study evidence using eye-tracking and video analysis. Autism, 16(1), 75-86.

Green, G. (1994). The quality of the evidence. In H. C. Shane (Ed.), Facilitated Communication. The Clinical and Social Phenomenon (pp. 157-225). San Diego: Singular.

*Heckler, S. (1994). Facilitated communication: A response by child protection. Child Abuse & Neglect, 18(6), 495-503.

*Hör, C., & Klauß, T. (2009). Facilitated Communication - Manipulation oder Ermöglichung schulischer Integration? In U. Strasser, J. Weisser, M. Wolters Kohler, M. Schmon, & J. Blickenstorfer (Eds.), Ästhetisierung der Sonderpädagogik. 44. Arbeitstagung der Sektion Sonderpädagogik der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Erziehungswissenschaften (DGfE) (pp. 283-299). Bad Heilbrunn: Klinkhardt.

*Howlin, P., & Jones, D. P. H. (1996). An assessment approach to abuse Aalegations made through facilitated communication. Child Abuse & Neglect, 20(2), 103-110.

*Hudson, A., Melita, B., & Arnold, N. (1993). Brief report: A case study assessing the validity of facilitated communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(1), 165-173.

International Society for Augmentative and Alternative Communication (ISAAC) (2014). Position Statement on Facilitated Communication. Retrieved on 16.12.2014 from http://valid.org.au/documents/isaac_fc_position_statement.pdf

*Janz, F., & Hör, C. (2009). Videoanalysen als Forschungsinstrument für die Untersuchung der Interaktion bei FC (Projekt KAFCA). In F. Janz & K. Terfloth (Eds.), Empirische Forschung im Kontext geistiger Behinderung (pp. 117-134). Heidelberg: Winter.

*Janz, F., & Klauß, T. (2012). Facilitated Communication - Interaktionsanalysen bei einer kontrovers diskutierten Methode. Heidelberg: Winter.

*Janzen-Wilde, M. L., Duchan, J. F., & Higginbotham, D. J. (1995). Successful Use of Facilitated Communication With an Oral Child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38(3), 658-676.

*Kerrin, R. G., Murdock, J. Y., Sharpton, W. R., & Jones, N. (1998). Who's Doing the Pointing? Investigating Facilitated Communication in a Classroom Setting with Students with Autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 13(2), 73-79.

*Kezuka, E. (1997). The Role of Touch in Facilitated Communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(5), 571-593.

*Klauß, T., Janz, F., & Hör, C. (2009). Was geschieht bei der >>Facilitated Communication<Sonderpädagogische Förderung heute, 54(1), 72-95.

*Klewe, L. (1993). Brief Report: An Empirical Evaluation of Spelling Boards as a Means of Communication for the Multihandicapped. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(3), 559-566.

*Koch, A., Lang, M., & Noebel, B. (2011). Überprüfung des Leseverständnisses von Schülern mit autistischen Verhaltensweisen im Kontext der Gestützten Kommunikation. In autismus Deutschland e.V. (Ed.), Inklusion von Menschen mit Autismus (pp. 188-199). Karlsruhe: Von Loeper.

*Köhler, J. M., & Scholz, M. (2014). Gestützte Kommunikation (FC) im Spannungsfeld wissenschaftlicher Erkenntnisse und praktischer Umsetzung. Eine Studie zur Verwendung einer umstrittenen Methode an Schulen in Rheinland-Pfalz. Heilpädagogische Forschung, 40(3), 133-141.

*Konstantareas, M. M. (1998). Allegations of sexual abuse by nonverbal autistic people via facilitated communication: testing of validity. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22(10), 1027-1041.

*Konstantareas, M. M., & Gravelle, G. (1998). Facilitated Communication. The contribution of physical, emotional and mental support. Autism, 2(4), 389-414.

*Lang, M. (2003). Auswirkung von Gestützter Kommunikation auf die Situation der Eltern. Geistige Behinderung, 2(2003), 148-154.

Lilienfeld, S. O., Marshall, J., Todd, J. T., & Shane, H. C. (2014). The persistence of fad interventions in the face of negative scientific evidence: Facilitated communication for autism as a case example. Evidence-based Communication Assessment and Intervention, 8(2), 62-101.

*Montee, B. B., Miltenberger, R. G., & Wittrock, D. (1995). An experimental analysis of facilitated communication. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28(2), 189-200.

*Moore, S., Donovan, B., & Hudson, A. (1993). Brief Report: Facilitator-Suggested Conversational Evaluation of Facilitated Communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(3), 541-552.

*Moore, S., Donovan, B., Hudson, A., Dykstra, J., & Lawrence, J. (1993). Brief report: Evaluation of eight case studies of facilitated communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(3), 531-539.

Mostert, M. P. (2001). Facilitated Communication Since 1995: A Review of Published Studies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(3), 287-313.

Mostert, M. P. (2010). Facilitated Communication and Its Legitimacy - Twenty-First Century Developments. Exceptionality, 18(1), 31-41.

Mostert, M. P. (2012). Facilitated communication: The empirical imperative to prevent further professional malpractice. Evidence-based Communication Assessment and Intervention, 6(1), 18-27.

*Myles, B. S., Simpson, R. L., & Smith, S. M. (1996a). Collateral Behavioral and Social Effects of Using Facilitated Communication with Individuals with Autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 11(3), 163-169.

*Myles, B. S., Simpson, R. L., & Smith, S. M. (1996b). Impact of Facilitated Communication Combined with Direct Instruction in Academic Performance of Individuals with Autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 11(1), 37-44.

*Niemi, J., & Kärnä-Lin, E. (2002). Grammar and Lexicon in Facilitated Communication: A Linguistic Authorship Analysis of a Finnish Case. Mental Retardation, 40(5), 347-357.

*Ogletree, B. T., Hamtil, A., Solberg, L., & Scoby-Schmelzle, S. (1993). Facilitated Communication: Illustration of a Naturalistic Validation Method. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 8(4), 1-10.

*Olney, M. (1995a). Reading Between the Lines: A Case Study on Facilitated Communication. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps (JASH), 20(1), 57-65.

*Olney, M. (1995b). Time and Task Sampling Approach to Validation: A Quantitative Evaluation of Facilitated Communication Using Educational Computer Games. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, San Francisco, CA.

*Olney, M. (1997). A Controlled Study of Facilitated Communication Using Computer Games. In D. Biklen & D. N. Cardinal (Eds.), Contested Words, Contested Science. Unraveling the Facilitated Communication controversy (pp. 96-114). New York: Teachers' College Press.

*Oswald, D. P. (1994). Facilitator Influence in Facilitated Communication. Journal of Behavioral Education, 4(2), 191-200.

*Perry, A., Bebko, J., & Bryson, S. (1994). Letter to the editor: Validity of facilitated communication: Failure to replicate Calculator & Singer (1992). Topics in Language Disorders, 14(4), 79-82.

*Perry, A., Bryson, S., & Bebko, J. (1998). Brief Report: Degree of Facilitator Influence in Facilitated Communication as a Function of Facilitator Characteristics, Attitudes, and Beliefs. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(1), 87-90.

Probst, P. (2005). "Communication unbound – or unfound?" - Ein integratives Literatur-Review zur Wirksamkeit der „Gestützten Kommunikation“ („Facilitated Communication/FC“) bei nichtsprechenden autistischen und intelligenzgeminderten Personen. Zeitschrift für klinische Psychologie, Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 53(2), 93-128.

*Probst, P. (2012a). Bericht über die Einstellungen von Fachverbänden und Selbsthilfevereinigungen zur Gestützten Kommunikation: Ergebnisse einer schriftlichen Befragung. Heilpädagogische Forschung, 38(3), 138-144.

*Probst, P. (2012b). Evaluative Studie zur Gestützten Kommunikation auf der Grundlage dreier Einzelfallanalysen an Personen mit Intelligenzminderung und Autismus-Spektrum-Störung. Heilpädagogische Forschung, 38(3), 110-127.

*Regal, R. A., Rooney, J. R., & Wandas, T. (1994). Facilitated Communication: An Experimental Evaluation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(3), 345-355.

*Remington-Gurney, J., & Crossley, R. (1990). Facilitated Communication in Mainstream Schools. Paper presented at the 4th Conference of the International Society on Augmentative and Alternative Communication, Stockholm. Retrieved on 06.10.2014 from http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED331267.pdf

*Sabin, L. A., & Donnellan, A. M. (1993). A Qualitative Study of the Process of Facilitated Communication. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps (JASH), 18(3), 200-211.

*Saloviita, T., Leppänen, M., & Ojalammi, U. (2014). Authorship in Facilitated Communication: An Analysis of 11 Cases. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 30(7) [Epub ahead of print]), 1-13.

*Saloviita, T., & Sariola, H. (2003). Authorship in Facilitated Communication: A Re-Analysis of a Case of Assumed Representative Authentic Writing. Mental Retardation, 41(5), 374-379.

*Schmalenbach, B. (1998). Gestützte Kommunikation im Spiegel der Texte. Seelenpflege in Heilpädagogik und Sozialtherapie, 17(1), 14-27.

*Schmalenbach, B. (2011). Gestützte Kommunikation und Lebenswelt. In A. D. e.V. (Ed.), Inklusion von Menschen mit Autismus (pp. 170-187). Karlsruhe: Von Loeper.

*Schubert, A. (1997). "I Want to Talk Like Everyone": On the Use of Multiple Means of Communication. Mental Retardation, 35(5), 347-354.

*Shane, H. C., & Kearns, K. (1994). An Examination of the Role of the Facilitator in "Facilitated Communication". American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 1994(3), 48-54.

*Sheehan, C. M., & Matuozzi, R. T. (1996). Investigation of the Validity of Facilitated Communication Trough the Disclosure of Unknown Information. Mental Retardation, 34(2), 94-107.

*Siegel, B. (1995). Brief Report: Assessing Allegations of Sexual Molestation Made Trough Facilitated Communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25(3), 319-326.

*Simon, E. W., Toll, D. M., & Whitehair, P. M. (1994). A naturalistic approach to the validation of facilitated communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 647-657.

*Simon, E. W., Whitehair, P. M., & Toll, D. M. (1996). A Case Study: Follow-Up Assessment of Facilitated Communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(1), 9-18.

*Sipilä, A.-K., & Määttä, K. (2011). Can the Facilitated Communication Method support Autistic People, according to Facilitators Opinions. Psychology Of Language and Communication, 15(1), 1-26.

*Smith, M. D., & Belcher, R. G. (1993). Brief Report: Facilitated Communication with Adults with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(1), 175-183.

*Smith, M. D., Haas, P. J., & Belcher, R. G. (1994). Facilitated communication: The effects of facilitator knowledge and level of assistance on output. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(3), 357-367.

*Szempruch, J., & Jacobson, J. W. (1993). Evaluating Facilitated Communications of People With Developmental Disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 14(4), 253-264.

*Trost, R. (2010). Verbreitung und Nutzung von Gestützter Kommunikation in den Schulen Baden-Württembergs. In A. Alfaré, T. Huber-Kaiser, F. Janz, & T. Klauß (Eds.), Facilitated Communication. Forschung und Praxis im Dialog (pp. 150-171). Karlsruhe: Von Loeper.

*Tuzzi, A. (2009). Grammar and Lexicon in Individuals With Autism: A Quantitative Analysis of a Large Italian Corpus. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 47(5), 373-385.

*Tuzzi, A., Cemin, M., & Castagna, M. (2004). "Moved deeply I am". Autistic language in texts produced with FC. In G. Purnelle, C. Fairon, & A. Dister (Eds.), JADT 2004. 7es Journées internationales d'Analyse statistique des Données Textuelles (pp. 1097-1105). Retrieved on 06.10.2014 from http://lexicometrica.univ-paris3.fr/jadt/jadt2004/pdf/JADT_108.pdf

*Twachtman-Cullen, D. (1997). A Passion to Believe. Autism and the Facilitated Communication Phenomenon. Boulder: Westview Press.

*Vázquez, C. A. (1994). Brief Report: A Multitask Controlled Evaluation of Facilitated Communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(3), 369-379.

*Vázquez, C. A. (1995). Failure to Confirm the Word-Retrieval Problem Hypothesis in Facilitated Communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25(6), 597-610.

*Wagner, M., & Kannewischer, S. (2012). Einschätzung der Kompetenzen im Bereich Sprache/Kommunikation. In W. Dworschak, S. Kannewischer, C. Ratz, & M. Wagner (Eds.), Schülerschaft mit dem Förderschwerpunkt geistige Entwicklung (SFGE). Eine empirische Studie (pp. 99-110). Oberhausen: Athena.

*Wegner, D. M., Fuller, V. A., & Sparrow, B. (2003). Clever Hands: Uncontrolled Intelligence in Facilitated Communication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 5-19.

*Weiss, M. J. S., & Wagner, S. H. (1997). Emerging Validation of Facilitated Communication: New Findings About Old Assumptions. In D. Biklen & D. N. Cardinal (Eds.), Contested Words, Contested Science. Unraveling the Facilitated Communication controversy (pp. 135-156). New York: Teachers' College Press.

*Wheeler, D. L., Jacobson, J. W., Paglieri, R. A., & Schwartz, A. A. (1992). An Experimental Assessment of Facilitated Communication. Retrieved on 06.10.2014 from http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED360797.pdf



Contributors

Markus Scholz
Department of Special Education, Ludwigsburg University of Education

Jan Markus Stegkemper
Department of Special Education, University Koblenz-Landau

Author Note
Markus Scholz, Department of Special Education, Ludwigsburg University of Education; Jan Markus Stegkemper, Department of Special Education, University Koblenz-Landau. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jan Markus Stegkemper, Institut für Sonderpädagogik, Universität Koblenz-Landau, Xylanderstraße 1, D-76829 Landau, Germany.

Corresponding Author: stegkemper@uni-landau.de